Pavlov in the Classroom
Materials: ★☆☆ Easy to get from supermarket or hardware store
Difficulty: ★☆☆ Can be easily done by most teenagers
Safety: ★★☆ Some safety precautions required to perform safely
Categories: Psychology
Alternative titles: Classical Conditioning
Summary
This demonstration shows how a neutral classroom cue (for example, a chime or clap pattern) can become a conditioned stimulus that elicits a predictable student response after repeated pairings with an instruction or routine. It connects Pavlov’s classical conditioning to practical classroom management by highlighting acquisition, extinction, and generalization.
Procedure
- Choose a neutral cue that students do not already associate with instructions (for example, a two-note chime or a unique clap pattern).
- Define a simple, observable target response (for example, eyes on teacher, voices off, hands still).
- Explain to the class that when they hear the cue, they should perform the target response. Practice once with coaching.
- Begin acquisition: run 6-10 short trials across a lesson. Give the cue, immediately give brief praise and a clear instruction that follows the response (for example, “Thank you. Open your notebooks to page 12.”).
- Keep the delay between the cue and the next action very short so the cue predicts what comes next.
- Record whether the class meets the target within 3 seconds of the cue on each trial to track learning across time.
- Test generalization: later in the day, use the same cue during a different activity and check whether the response occurs without coaching.
- Demonstrate extinction: run two or three instances where the cue is given but no instruction follows and no praise is provided. Note how the conditioned response weakens.
- Demonstrate recovery: after a short break, present the cue again with praise and instruction to show rapid return of the response.
- Debrief students: connect what they experienced to classical conditioning terms (neutral stimulus, unconditioned stimulus, conditioned stimulus, conditioned response, acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery).
Links
Ivan Pavlov's Classical Conditioning Psychology Experiment - Andrew Daughters:
📄 Pavlov in the Classroom: Understanding Triggers and Responses - Teach HQ: https://teachhq.com/article/show/pavlov-in-the-classroom-understanding-triggers-and-responses
📄 Pavlov’s Dogs Experiment and Pavlovian Conditioning Response - Simple Psychology: https://www.simplypsychology.org/pavlov.html
Variations
- Pair the cue with different classroom routines (transition to groups, tidy up, start reading) to compare how quickly each becomes conditioned.
- Use visual cues (raised sign or hand signal) versus auditory cues (chime or clap) and compare which acquires faster.
- Compare immediate versus delayed follow-ups after the cue to show how timing affects conditioning strength.
- Have student leaders deliver the cue to test whether the conditioned response transfers to a different person.
- Use a clicker or bell for a single small group while the rest of the class works, to illustrate selective conditioning.
Safety Precautions
- Avoid loud or aversive sounds; keep cue volume comfortable to protect hearing.
- Do not use negative or fear-based stimuli; conditioning should remain positive and supportive.
- Obtain consent if collecting identifiable performance data; anonymize records if shared.
- Be culturally sensitive with gestures or signals to ensure inclusivity and comfort for all students.
- Ensure the cue does not disadvantage students with sensory processing differences; provide alternative accessible cues if needed.
Questions to Consider
- What turned the neutral cue into a conditioned stimulus? (Repeated pairing of the cue with immediate instructions and positive feedback.)
- Why did the response weaken during extinction? (Without reinforcement or the expected follow-up, the cue no longer predicted an outcome.)
- How does timing affect learning in classical conditioning? (Short intervals between cue and consequence strengthen the association.)
- Did the response generalize to new contexts or people? (If yes, the association transferred; if not, more varied practice is needed.)
- How can teachers keep conditioning positive rather than coercive? (Use brief, pleasant cues and reinforce desired behaviors with praise and clarity.)
- What classroom routines benefit most from conditioned cues? (Transitions and attention-getting often show the fastest, most reliable effects.)